J_Bus_Account_Financ_Perspect 2021, 3(1), 7; doi:10.35995/jbafp3010007
Cultural Perspectives of Financial and Non-Financial Incentives
Department of Economics, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, 10561 Athens, Greece; kkafka@econ.uoa.gr (K.K.); annmkanz@econ.uoa.gr (A.M.K.)
*
Corresponding author: ppetrak@econ.uoa.gr
How to cite: Kyriaki Kafka, Anna Maria Kanzola, Panagiotis E. Petrakis. Cultural Perspectives of Financial and Non-Financial Incentives. J. Bus. Account. Financ. Perspect., 2021, 3(1): 7; doi:10.35995/jbafp3010007.
Received: 31 January 2021 / Accepted: 4 March 2021 / Published: 10 March 2021
Abstract
:The present paper delineates an explanatory framework for the defining factors of incentives, both financial and nonfinancial, through the theory of human economic action and that of personality traits, which shape human goals and, ultimately, social identity. It is ascertained that three types of variables affect incentives: basic conditions (cultural change, etc.), basic values and needs (tradition, external values, etc.) and the dynamism of social identity, which includes the goals that are set. More specifically, the two basic variables that shape the incentives for human action and imbue dynamism in behavior relate to megalothymia—i.e., the need for acknowledgement of a person’s integrity along with the predisposition to be thought superior to others as well as the aspiration to a certain level of quality in life.
Keywords:
culture; financial incentives; non-financial incentives; human economic action1. Introduction
Incentives shape a person’s method of action. Consequently, when speaking of basic human incentives in the field of economics, we refer to personal financial decisions, to the assessment of the future in relation to the present (savings vs. investment), business decisions (investment, budgeting, financing, allocation of dividends) and issues to do with collective courses of action, taxation and the role of the state. Human incentives are deeper human forces shaping behaviors without a directly observable relation between incentives and the final (financial) decisions mentioned above.
It is difficult to locate direct causal relations between incentives and financial outcomes. Thus, a nonfinancial incentive (i.e., megalothymia) can eventually be the motivating force for making a financial decision (i.e., an investment decision) as can the subject’s aspiration to become wealthier, which could be considered as a a purely financial incentive. In approaching the nature of human incentives, we could start by saying that cultural dimensions (of society and the individual) shape the values of life (natural, internal, normal) which in turn affect the formation of those incentives.
The paper’s aim, initially, is to examine what the basic forces that affect human economic action are, what constitutes the individual’s behavioral identity—and, by extension, their social identity—and how these relate to human incentives. In this context, social identity includes a range of aspects such as the person’s perception about environmental security, the basic elements of social identity (megalothymia, religion, altruism, etc.), demographics and financial behavior. Subsequently, the extent and the ways in which social identity affects the formation of human incentives are addressed.
The empirical data concern Greek society in the 2019–2020 period. We treat the two years as one. It is noted on the basis of closely allied research (Petrakis et al., 2021) that the basic behaviors of individuals within the two years do not significantly vary, evidently because the appearance of COVID-19 in 2020 is considered a temporary, exogenous event.
The main conclusion is that particular traits of social identity do indeed shape human incentives. This finding raises questions regarding policymaking that could be put in place in order to influence the operation of the economy and financial decision-making.
The paper’s structure is as follows: in part 2, the basic traits of human economic behavior are described as well as how social identity is formed. This part also explores how the relation is organized between, on the one hand, human behavior and social identity and, on the other, human incentives. In part 3, the selection of variables is analyzed. In part 4, the methodology and the empirical findings are presented. Finally, in part 5, there is a discussion of the evidence.
2. Human Behavior, Social Identity and Incentives
The economic thinking on how incentives come about concerns the operation of the economy and centers on expected utility theory (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979) which in turn is based on rational choice (Briggs, 2014). Thus, the “motives” of human behavior and the “goals” of human beings are revealed.
Not all individual motives follow a strictly optimizing/maximizing tendency, nor, of course, does everyone possess the same ability—cognitive and otherwise—to analyze information about the state of the world. Even more importantly, it is not at all certain on the basis of the research undertaken that everyone agrees with the aspect of the maximization of utility or wealth. Additionally, competition and signaling in circulation in the markets and in society can create so much confusion that the forces of restoration to a state of balance may not be in a position to operate, or not within the requisite time period.
2.1. Human Economic Behavior and Personality Traits
Attempts to analyze the dimensions of social behaviors and the dynamic relations created within a society and its cultural background have been undertaken by researchers such as Hofstede (Hofstede, 1980) and Schwartz (Schwartz, 1994).
Through his research, Hofstede (Hofstede, 1980) launched the option of comparative analyses by initially proposing four cultural dimensions (power distance, uncertainty avoidance, individualism/collectivism, masculinity/femininity, and confucian dynamism) which describe how a society behaves, makes decisions and manages social issues. Subsequently, (Hofstede et al., 2010) added a fifth cultural dimension (indulgence/restraint).
Schwartz (Schwartz, 1994) mapped the cultural background of societies through seven different kinds of values (autonomy vs. embeddedness, intellectual autonomy vs. affective autonomy, egalitarianism vs. hierarchy and harmony vs. mastery). Through these, he attempted to formulate answers to questions on the individual’s relation to the group, environment and society, as well as a definition of collective responsibility.
Change of behaviors hinges on the prevailing social and economic conditions as well as on an evolutionary path dependence. Deaux and Snyder (Deaux and Snyder, 2012) note that social behaviors can be influenced either directly through individual traits that contribute to group cohesion—which is something that pertains to personality traits and social psychology—or indirectly, through the way the members of a group/society are connected and the characteristics of the group’s cohesion.
In the early 1930s, a first attempt was made by McDougall (McDougall, 1932) to structure the concept of personality. McDougall (McDougall, 1932) delineated five factors which may satisfactorily account for personality. These are: intellect, character, temperament, disposition and temper. Each of these factors is distinct and of particular complexity.
The most prevailing view about the characteristics of the personality is the Big Five Personality Traits Hypothesis (Digman, 1990) and this may either concern the personality itself or the person’s professional profile, as this is determined in the workplace. The hypothesis is based on the Five-Factor Model (FFM), which is made up of the following factors: openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism.
It is noted that personality traits may occupy opposite poles and, basically, are present in the composition of the individual personality in different percentages. Overall, external conditions change the analogies of the bipolar correlations so that a different prevailing portfolio of personality traits can emerge. At a collective level, we consider of particular interest the degree of convergence between the personalities of individuals that leads to their categorization in groups.
2.2. Social Identity
Social identity theory focuses on the way social behaviors are formed and how they affect the individual’s perceptions—i.e., how we are led from social structure to social behaviors. Thus, the theory of social identity (Tajfel, 1982) concerns the prevailing social values and beliefs and, as such, it is affected by both endogenous and exogenous factors. Fukuyama (Fukuyama, 2018) analyzes personal identity by means of the thymos and integrity. However, individual identity is incorporated in social identity through the assumption of roles within society (Stryker and Burke, 2000). This takes place through people’s self-categorization (Oakes et al., 1987).
Identity can now provide an explanatory framework for the shaping of human action. Indeed, personal and social identity interact and mutually shape one another (Kanzola and Petrakis, 2021). Social identity encompasses emotional factors and has a bearing on psychological state (Turner et al., 1987) and it may also include personal identity (Hogg et al., 1995).
Through the attribute of salience (Stets and Burke, 2000), identities can mobilize in order to influence and direct social situations/outcomes (Oakes et al., 1987; Stryker and Burke, 2000). Of course, not all parts/aspects of an identity are mobilized, and which part does so depends on the particular circumstances. So, certain circumstances (such as, for instance, a sense of danger) can certainly mobilize aspects of the personality. In identity theory, the concepts of mobilization and salience tend, overall, to overlap (Kanzola and Petrakis, 2021).
2.3. Goals and Incentives
Social organization takes place under the influence of elements (unifying and not) that determine human action. Motives in the form of incentives are affected by internal needs and are thus not observable but, rather, the manifestation of the person’s inner processes which aim to satisfy their goals and needs. Motives are springboards for human action and provide an explanation to the question why do people behave the way they do? In order to provide further elaboration, we enrich the answer with the theory of identity. In general, we observe the external manifestations of motives, not the motives themselves (Petrakis, 2020).
There is an overall agreement nowadays that motives are directed by the goals set by human beings (Lawler, 1994) and that they affect the efforts to attain these goals. A goal or an aim is considered a future state which one endeavors to attain (Kruglanski, 1996). Goals can be differentiating factors for individuals, both in terms of their kind and the strategies required to attain them. As has been mentioned, goals are the outcome of motives which, in turn, are generated by needs (Petrakis, 2011) and, thus, we are led to a cyclical process of interactions. It is ascertained that the organization of goals is necessary for the study of motives (Ford, 1992) which, according to Freud (Freud, 1915) may derive from the conscious or the unconscious parts of the personality.
Goals fall under a single category of knowledge which possesses its own primary characteristics. From there on, the variety of phenomena related to goal setting fall under three levels of reading. The first and most general refers to the organization of cognitive constructs which make up a personal trait, but are also diffuse in culture. Thus, the same principles which underline the accumulation, modification and mobilization of the cognitive constructs (hence, of stereotypes) are also valid for the goals which are set. The second level refers to particular psychological traits/attributes and psychological functions. Finally, the third concerns specific goals and types of goals, insofar as not all goals are the same or possess the same degree of significance.
Goal setting as a cognitive category is realized through a process of judgment, which is affected by the mobilization of logical constructs and stereotypes existing in memory, and motives. Responsibility for the organization of goals, however, does not only fall under the conscious part of the mind, but also the unconscious. The mobilization of mental constructs takes place through the process of priming, where a previous stimulus affects the manner of the individual’s mobilization. Correspondingly, a previous goal affects a subsequent one, and so on.
We arrive at the realization that, while motives are an individual state, the goals that are set relate directly to cultural background (common knowledge). Basically, goals link cultural stereotypes with individual behaviors. In this sense, they are extremely important for social and economic processes and economic development and growth. Moreover, goals, by definition, concern future states for the individual. If, for any reason, there is no temporal horizon in the operation of individuals and societies, then, essentially, the interaction is cancelled between incentives and cultural background.
3. Selection Variables
In order to measure the concept of social identity, an approach based on detailed interviews was taken (Kanzola and Petrakis, 2021). The major issue is the wording of the questions used in the field research, which are the outcome of an interdisciplinary approach to the fields shaping social identity.
In Table 1, the variables are presented that were used to measure social identity, followed by the reasoning for selecting these particular variables. The questions describing social identity are equal to 24 in number. They fall into two sections: in the first section, the degree of satisfaction and the personal situation are presented. In the second section, are included three subsections: basic traits of the respondent, traits to do with life attitude and demographic information.
In the first section, which is concerned with the degree of satisfaction regarding life and personal situation, matters are included to do with the level of happiness (Haslam et al., 2009) and satisfaction with life (Wakefield et al., 2017), but also the person’s state of health (Haslam et al., 2009). At the same time, the feeling of security (Theiler, 2003) is considered since it forms a prerequisite for social life to thrive. Additionally, people who claim to feel safe are expected to acknowledge the importance of disciplinary rules (Smyth et al., 2015) such as the educational character of socialization agencies, and the importance of external values (McGowan et al., 2017) such as consistency in following instructions by one’s superiors.
As regards the contents of the second section, and specifically the basic traits of identity, here are included the trends of egalitarianism (Grantham, 2013; Jetten et al., 2017; Luria and Kaufman, 2017) and of megalothymia (Danilova, 2016; Mehta et al., 2017)—described by Fukuyama (Fukuyama, 2018)—which are also basic traits of social identity since they refer to ways in which a person defines themself in relation to their environment. Moreover, a common religion (Greil and Davidman, 2007) and adherence to traditions (Hayek, 1958) are included as major aspects of social cohesion. In general, once the means of earning a livelihood have been secured, then the incentives are present for the person to pursue felicity and having a good time (Wakefield et al., 2017).
As regards the life attitude, the matter is taken into consideration in the change in national identity (Fuligni and Flook, 2005; Jaspal, 2012)—i.e., the redefinition of cultural values (Hamamura, 2016; Reicher, 2004)—which may explain behaviors to do with the role of the migrants. Political self-position is used (Huddy and Bankert, 2017; Huddy and Mason, 2010) which also influences views on the role of the state in economic development (Hornung et al., 2019) as it touches on policies of wealth distribution and the welfare system. People with shared educational backgrounds may show similarities in their dispositions to protect the environment (Brieger, 2019) and to offer help to their fellow men selflessly (altruism) (Fowler and Kam, 2007).
In tandem with altruism, the degree of trust (Güth et al., 2008) people have in their fellow men is important. Social identity includes the tendency for creativity and to doing things in one’s own way (Petrakis and Kafka, 2016), which in turn relates to freedom in decision making. Additionally, the existence of friends makes a difference for the person, in the sense that they could borrow money if they found themselves in a difficult financial situation.
Finally, in terms of demographic characteristics, income status (Jetten et al., 2017), age (Zacher et al., 2018) as a factor indicative of the level of maturity, and the level of education (Kelly, 2009) are included, as a person’s training and schooling affect their beliefs and attitudes about the world and their relationships with others.
Regarding incentives, it emerges that they play a determining role in individual performance (Cameron and Pierce, 1994; Jenkins et al., 1998) and they do so without having a negative impact on intrinsic motivation (Deci and Ryan, 1985; Kohn, 1993; Pfeffer, 1998). Cerasoli et al. (Cerasoli et al., 2014) adopted a wider approach, scanning the bibliography for the psychological, educational, medical and entrepreneurial sectors, specifically focusing on intrinsic motives.
Cameron and Pierce (Cameron and Pierce, 1994) examined the role of financial incentives and concluded that, in the absence of financial incentives, the relation between intrinsic motives and performance was positive and moderately significant. When financial incentives were offered, the relation between intrinsic motives and performance was strengthened. They thus observed that the validity of intrinsic motives was not reduced but strengthened in the presence of financial incentives, boosting performance.
Other studies link financial incentives with matters to do with health coverage (Giles et al., 2014) and psychology (Byron and Khazanchi, 2012). Giles et al., (Giles et al., 2014) conclude that financial incentives relate to a range of functional behaviors to do with health, such as the cessation of smoking (short term and long term) and compliance with vaccination. Byron and Khazanchi (Byron and Khazanchi, 2012), found that individuals receiving incentives that were linked to creative performance were indeed more effective. In the present study, the determination of human incentives is attempted through a series of variables presented in Table 2.
For the selection of the variables expressing the incentives, it is argued in the present paper that financial incentives are present in the analysis, although, naturally, people are motivated by other factors as well.
The variable of surprises is used in the sense that people wish to continuously have new experiences and be in new situations in order to increase the chances of attaining life satisfaction (Chughtai, 2019), which in turn leads to increased performances (Economic Policy Institute, 2000; Liu, 1980) through pleasure in what the person is doing. The incentive of success—which relates to acknowledgement of achievements so that, through historical continuity, the person attains new successes—is connected to financial performance (Kressler, 2003). Enjoying the respect of others definitely involves the strengthening of integrity and the individual’s personality and leads to the pursuit of high performances. The absence of others’ respect leads to lower per capita income.
Finally, the wish for income equalization is an indication of people’s position on whether this is the government’s responsibility (Bilan et al., 2020). The incentive of become wealthy is a clear indicator of an attraction (or aversion) to financial gain, wealth and prosperity (Ickowitz, 2020; Mehta et al., 2017; Tobin, 1965).
This paper is based on data emerging from responses to a questionnaire distributed in Greek society in 2019–2020. There were two different phases of gathering responses to the same questionnaire, by 800 people in 2019 and 800 people in 2020. To avoid historical idiosyncrasies related primarily to 2020 (due to COVID-19), the years were treated as one, increasing the number of available observations to 1600.
4. Methodology and Empirical Findings
The explanatory variables are fairly complex, so, for the purpose of simplification, Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was used, which reduces the number of variables describing behavioral identity, although not at the expense of the causal relations.
Observations showing extreme values with residual demands, such as “Don’t know/No response/Other”, were removed so that the full answers came to 1285 and the dimensions of the dataset were 1283 × 24. Hence, 24 components were extracted—as many as the variables used. The first five PCs represent approximately 50% of the overall data variation (Table 3). Table 3 presents the principal component analysis for the case of social identity.
Correspondingly, Table 4 presents the principal component analysis of the part variables.
It was found that the first two PCs represent 60% of data variation. The issue then was to ascertain the explanatory power of the social identity PCs in relation to the PCs of the financial incentives. To this end, we ran a regression using the 24 PCs of social identity against the first and most representative of the PCs of the incentives. It was found that in PC1 the five variables with the highest loadings (>0.30) are: surprises, to be successful, to draw pleasure from one’s activities, and the incentive to become wealthy and to earn the respect of others.
The analysis may be further pursued so as to locate those variables of social identity that define each of the part incentives that we have identified. Thus, we turned to the statistically significant PCs of social identity and we choose from among them the variables with a loading >0.30. We used these statistically significant variables as independent variables to interpret the five incentives above (Table 5, Table 6, Table 7, Table 8 and Table 9).
The results regarding the degree of explanatory value of the part variables of the incentives are shown in Table 10.
It was found that the social identity of Greek society shapes incentives so that they mainly concern postmaterialistic rather than materialistic contents (Norris and Inglehart, 2019). From analysis of the econometric results for the formation of part incentives, it is seen that the incentive of success is determined by the positive stability of tradition and the negative influence of the external values. Megalothymia and having a good time are also found to be determining factors of success.
In deriving pleasure from one’s activities, care for the environment plays a part while the variables megalothymia, having a good time and positivity towards cultural change are also present.
The incentive of become wealthy is negatively influenced by political self-position and tradition, as well as by trust and friendship, while acknowledgement of external values has a positive influence. The incentive of surprises is negatively influenced by happiness, and positively by environment, megalothymia and the desire to have a good time.
5. Discussion of the Evidence: What Determines the Incentives
As was previously ascertained, the roles of cultural background and, more importantly, of social identity can be extremely important in shaping the nexus of incentives of human economic behavior. Thus, we introduced the concept of cultural dimensions as they manifest in social identity in order to analyze the factors shaping financial incentives.
A close interconnection has been theoretically ascertained between human incentives, needs and goals. The question which this paper has set out to answer is whether the empirical data confirm these theoretical causalities. In processing the econometric results, we are in a position to observe that all the explanatory variables of the incentives may be set within three basic groups: basic conditions, basic values (needs) and dynamism of behaviors (goals) (Table 11).
In sum, we see that the forces influencing the formation of incentives fall into three categories which correspond to the theoretical predictions that have been described; we see presence of the basic conditions and, more importantly, we also observe the presence of needs (basic values) and goals (dynamism).
The basic conditions are shaped by three variables: happiness (with a negative value), political self-position and the predisposition towards cultural change. The state of happiness enters into pleasure in one’s activities and the quest for surprises. Cultural change relates to pleasure in one’s activities. Thus, when we are happy, we draw less pleasure from what we do and we also seek surprises less often. Additionally, the more to the political right a person places themselves, the much higher is their motive to become wealthy. Additionally, the positive predisposition towards cultural change is a necessary ingredient of basic conditions.
Basic values correspond to human needs and are five in number. They are tradition, the environment, the acknowledgement of external values, trust and friendship. Tradition expresses the stability of values and affects the cultural boundaries along which incentives may develop. A caring attitude towards the environment is a basic principle of life and the acknowledgement of external values makes for the institutional boundaries along which incentives may develop. For a human being to draw pleasure from what they do, this needs to combine with a caring attitude towards the environment. Trust and friendship affect the framework in which incentives develop.
Tradition has a positive value, and the acknowledgement of external values has a negative one. The incentive of become wealthy is interpreted negatively by the conditions of tradition, trust and friendship. In actual fact, the incentive of become wealthy is a positive motivating factor for the existence of external values, while the rest of the factors have soothing characters and, as such, they exert weakening effects. Finally, the environment is once again positively combined with the individual’s predisposition to encounter new surprises. It is worth noting the significance of tradition with a changeable value, depending on the incentive on each occasion, which is also the case for the acknowledgement of external values.
The category which is of special interest is that of dynamism which includes human goals. It contains two basic variables: megalothymia and the person’s predisposition to have a good time. This section contains only variables with positive values and it affects the formation of all incentives in the same way. Perhaps here is where the main generative force resides—i.e., the nucleus of the creation of human incentives.
In sum, the two basic goals which produce the incentives of human action and imbue human behaviors with dynamism are related to two basic human pursuits: megalothymia—i.e., the person’s need to have their dignity acknowledged; their predisposition to be considered superior to other people—and their tendency to have a good time—i.e., to have a satisfactory level of life. These goals along with the basic conditions and basic values (needs) give form to the cultural perspectives of human financial and nonfinancial incentives.
References
- Bilan, Y.; Halyna, M.; Natalia, S.; Halyna, Y. Impact of Income Distribution on Social and EconomicWell-Being of the State. Sustainability 2020, 12(1), 429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brieger, S. A. Social Identity and Environmental Concern: The Importance of Contextual Effects. Environment and Behavior 2019, 51(7), 828–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Briggs, R. Normative theories of rational choice: Expected utility. In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy; Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Byron, K.; Khazanchi, S. Rewards and creative performance: A meta-analytic test of theoretically derived hypotheses. Psychological Bulletin 2012, 138(4), 809–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cameron, J.; Pierce, W. D. Reinforcement, reward, and intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research 1994, 64(3), 363–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cerasoli, C. P.; Nicklin, J. M.; Ford, M. T. Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic incentives jointly predict performance: A 40-year meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 2014, 140(4), 980–1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chughtai, A. A. A Closer Look at the Relationship between Life Satisfaction and Job Performance. Applied Research in Quality of Life 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danilova, T. The Desire for Recognition in the context of Francis Fukuyama’s Universal History. Anthropological Measurements of Philosophical Research 2016, 69–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deaux, K.; Snyder, M. The Oxford handbook of personality and social psychology; Oxford University Press, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Deci, E. L.; Ryan, R. M. Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior; Plenum, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Digman, J. M. Personality Structure: Emergence of the Five-Factor Model. Annual Review of Psychology 1990, 41(1), 417–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Economic Policy Institute (EPI). The link between productivity growth and living standards. Snapshot 2000. https://www.epi.org/publication/webfeatures_snapshots_archive_03222000/. [Google Scholar]
- Ford, M. E. Motivating humans: Goals, emotions, and personal agency beliefs; Sage, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Fowler, J. H.; Kam, C. D. Beyond the Self: Social Identity, Altruism, and Political Participation. Journal of Politics 2007, 69(3), 813–827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freud, S. The unconscious. In The standard edition of the complete psychological works of sigmund freud, XIV (1914-1916): On the History of the Psycho-Analytic Movement, Papers on Metapsychology and Other Works; 1915; pp. 159–215. [Google Scholar]
- Fukuyama, F. Identity: The demand for dignity and the politics of resentment; Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2018; https://books.google.gr/books?id=OjpIDwAAQBAJ.
- Fuligni, A. J.; Flook, L. A social identity approach to ethnic differences in family relationships during adolescence. In Advances in Child Development and Behavior; Kail, R. V., Ed.; JAI, 2005; Vol. 33, pp. 125–152. [Google Scholar]
- Güth, W.; Levati, M. V.; Ploner, M. Social identity and trust—An experimental investigation. The Journal of Socio-Economics 2008, 37(4), 1293–1308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giles, E. L.; Robalino, S.; McColl, E.; Sniehotta, F. F.; Adams, J. The effectiveness of financial incentives for health behaviour change: systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2014, 9(3), e90347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grantham, T. Creativity and Equity: The Legacy of E. Paul Torrance as an Upstander for Gifted Black Males. The Urban Review 2013, 45, 518–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greil, A.; Davidman, L. Religion and identity. In The SAGE Handbook of the Sociology of Religion; SAGE Publications Ltd.: London, UK, 2007; pp. 549–565. [Google Scholar]
- Hamamura, T. Social identity and attitudes toward cultural diversity: A cultural psychological analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 2016, 48, 184–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haslam, S. A.; Jolanda, J.; Tom, P.; Haslam, C. Social Identity, Health and Well-Being: An Emerging Agenda for Applied Psychology. Applied Psychology 2009, 58(1), 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayek, F. A. Freedom, reason, and tradition. Ethics 1958, 68(4), 229–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofstede, G. Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values; Sage, 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Hofstede, G.; Hofstede, G. J.; Minkov, M. Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind, (Revised and expanded 3rd edtion); McGraw-Hill: NY, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Hogg, M. A.; Terry, D. J.; White, K. M. A Tale of Two Theories: A Critical Comparison of Identity Theory with Social Identity Theory. Social Psychology Quarterly 1995, 58(4), 255–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hornung, J.; Bandelow, N. C.; Vogeler, C. S. Social identities in the policy process. Policy Sciences 2019, 52(2), 211–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huddy, L.; Bankert, A. Political Partisanship as a Social Identity. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Politics. 2017. from https://oxfordre.com/politics/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228637-e-250 Retrieved 4 March 2021.
- Huddy, L.; Mason, L. Measuring partisanship as a social identity; Predicting Political Activism, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Ickowitz, A. The distribution of wealth and the Pace of Development, Studies in Development Strategy and Systemic Transformation; Palgrave MacMillan, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Jaspal, R. The construction of ethnic identity: Insights from identity process theory. Ethnicities 2012, 12, 503–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jenkins, J. G.; Mitra, A.; Gupta, N.; Shaw, J. Are Financial Incentives Related to Performance? A Meta-Analytic Review of Empirical Research. Journal of Applied Psychology 1998, 83, 777–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jetten, J.; Wang, Z.; Steffens, N. K.; Mols, F.; Peters, K.; Verkuyten, M. A social identity analysis of responses to economic inequality. Current Opinion in Psychology 2017, 18, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica 1979, 47(2), 263–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kanzola, A. M.; Petrakis, P. E. Τhe Sustainability of Creativity. Sustainability 2021, 13, 2776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelly, S. Social identity theories and educational engagement. British Journal of Sociology of Education 2009, 30(4), 449–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kohn, A. Punished by rewards: The trouble with gold stars, incentive plans, A’s, praise, and other bribes; Houghton, Mifflin and Company, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Kressler, H. Motivate and reward: Performance appraisal and incentive systems for business success. Human Resource Management Journal 2003, 14(1), 99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kruglanski, A. W. Motivated social cognition: Principles of the interface. In Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles; The Guilford Press, 1996; pp. 493–520. [Google Scholar]
- Lawler, E. E. Motivation in Work Organizations; Wiley, 1994; https://books.google.gr/books?id=oRdgQgAACAAJ.
- Liu, B.-C. Economic Growth and Quality of Life: A Comparative Indicator Analysis between China (Taiwan), U.S.A. and Other Developed Countries. The American Journal of Economics and Sociology 1980, 39(1), 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luria, S. R.; Kaufman, J. C. Examining the relationship between creativity and equitable thinking in schools. Psychology in the Schools 2017, 54(10), 1279–1284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDougall, W. Of the Words Character and Personality. Journal of Personality 1932, 1(1), 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGowan, M.; Shiu, E.; Hassan, L. M. The influence of social identity on value perceptions and intention. Journal of Consumer Behaviour 2017, 16(3), 242–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mehta, R.; Dahl, D. W.; Zhu, R. Social-Recognition versus Financial Incentives? Exploring the Effects of Creativity-Contingent External Rewards on Creative Performance. Journal of Consumer Research 2017, 44(3), 536–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Norris, P.; Inglehart, R. Cultural Backlash: Trump, Brexit, and authoritarian populism; Cambridge University Press, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Oakes, P.; Turner, J.; Hogg, M.; Reicher, S.; Wetherell, M. Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory; Basil Blackwell, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Petrakis, P. E. The Greek Economy and the Crisis. Challenges and Responses; Springer: New York and Heidelberg, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Petrakis, P. E. Theoretical approaches to economic growth and development—An interdisciplinary perspective; Palgrave MacMillan, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Petrakis, P. E.; Kafka, K. I. Entrepreneurial Creativity and Growth; Intech, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Petrakis, P. E.; Kafka, K. I.; Kostis, P. C.; Valsamis, D. G. Greek Culture after the Financial Crisis: An Economic Analysis; Palgrave MacMillan, 2021; (Accepted to be publishes). [Google Scholar]
- Pfeffer, J. Seven Practices of Successful Organizations. California Management Review 1998, 40(2), 97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reicher, S. The Context of Social Identity: Domination, Resistance, and Change. Political Psychology 2004, 25(6), 921–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, S. H. Beyond individualism/collectivism: New cultural dimensions of values. In Cross-Cultural Research and Methodology Series; APA: Washington, DC, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Smyth, L.; Mavor, K. I.; Platow, M. J.; Grace, D. M.; Reynolds, K. J. Discipline social identification, study norms and learning approach in university students. Educational Psychology 2015, 35(1), 53–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stets, J. E.; Burke, P. J. Identity Theory and Social Identity Theory. Social Psychology Quarterly 2000, 63(3), 224–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stryker, S.; Burke, P. J. The Past, Present, and Future of an Identity Theory. Social Psychology Quarterly 2000, 63(4), 284–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tajfel, H. Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Annual Review of Psychology 1982, 33(1), 1–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Theiler, T. Societal Security and Social Psychology. Review of International Studies 2003, 29, 249–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tobin, J. Money and Economic Growth. Econometrica 1965, 33, 671–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, J. C.; Hogg, M. A.; Oakes, P. J.; Reicher, S. D.; Wetherell, M. S. Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory; Basil Blackwell, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Wakefield, J. R. H.; Sani, F.; Madhok, V.; Norbury, M.; Dugard, P.; Gabbanelli, C.; Arnetoli, M.; Beconcini, G.; Botindari, L.; Grifoni, F.; Paoli, P.; Poggesi, F. The Relationship Between Group Identification and Satisfaction with Life in a Cross-Cultural Community Sample. Journal of Happiness Studies 2017, 18(3), 785–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zacher, H.; Esser, L.; Bohlmann, C.; Rudolph, C. Age, social identity and identification, and work outcomes: A conceptual model, literature review, and future research directions. Work Aging and Retirement 2018, 5, 24–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variable | Question | Scale of Answers and Quantification | |
---|---|---|---|
1. Degree of Satisfaction and Personal Situation | |||
1 | Happiness | Overall, how happy would you say you are? | Scale from “Perfectly Unhappy” to “Perfectly Happy” |
(Scale 0 to 10) | |||
2 | Satisfaction | As an overall appraisal, how satisfied are you generally with your life today? | Scale from “Perfectly Dissatisfied” to “Perfectly Satisfied” |
(Scale 0 to 10) | |||
3 | Sate of Health | What would you say is your state of health overall? Would you say it is… | Very good, good, medium, bad, very bad |
(1,2,3,4,5, respectively) | |||
4 | Security | It is important for the respondent to live in a safe environment. | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
2. Identity Traits | |||
2.1. Basic Traits | |||
5 | Egalitarianism | The respondent believes that it is important that all people in the world should be treated equally. | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
6 | Megalothymia | It is important for the respondent to show their capabilities. | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
7 | Religion | Irrespectively of whether you belong to a particular religion, how religious would you say you are on a scale of 0 to 10? | Scale from “Not at all religious” to “Very religious” |
(Scale from 0 to 10) | |||
8 | Tradition | Tradition is important to the respondent. | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
9 | Adherence to rules | Schools need to teach children to adhere to rules and be disciplined. | Strongly agree, Agree, Neither agree nor disagree, Disagree, Strongly disagree |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
10 | Acknowledgment of external values | The respondent believes that people should do as they are told. | Strongly agree, Agree, Agree somewhat, Agree a little, Disagree, Strongly disagree |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
11 | Having a good time | It’s important for the respondent to have a good time. | Strongly agree, Agree, Agree somewhat, Agree a little, Disagree, Strongly disagree |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
2.2. Life Attitude | |||
12 | The role of migrants | The presence of migrants in our country enriches our culture | Agree, Probably agree, Probably disagree, Disagree |
(1,2,3,4, respectively) | |||
13 | Economic Development, The role of the state | The less the government intervenes in the economy, the better for the country. | Strongly agree, Agree, Neither agree nor disagree, Disagree, Strongly disagree |
(1,2,3,4,5, respectively) | |||
14 | Cultural Identity | Is the cultural life of Greece overall downgraded or enriched by people coming here to live from other countries? | Scale from “Cultural life is downgraded” to “Cultural life is enriched” |
(Scale from 0 to 10) | |||
15 | Altruism | Is it important for the respondent to help people around them? | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
16 | Environment | Firmly believes that people should take care of nature | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
17 | Trust | Generally speaking, would you say we can trust most people or should we always be cautious? | Scale from “We must always be cautious” to “We can trust most people” |
(Scale from 0 to 10) | |||
18 | Political Self-position | In politics, people sometimes speak of “Left” and “Right”. Where would you place yourself? | Scale from “Left” to “Right” |
(Scale from 0 to 10) | |||
19 | Creativity | It is important for the respondent to have new ideas and to be creative. They like doing things in their own, original way. | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important (1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) |
20 | Freedom | It is important for the respondent to make their own decisions for whatever they do. They like to be free and not need to depend on others. | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
21 | Friendship | If for some reason, you were in a difficult situation financially and had to borrow money to cover your obligations, how hard or easy would it be to do so? | Extremely hard, Fairly hard, So and so, Fairly easy, Extremely easy |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
2.3. Demographics | |||
22 | Income Status | In which of the following categories does the monthly income of your household belong after tax? | <700 euro, 701–1000 euro, 1001–1250 euro, 1251–1500 euro, 1501–1750 euro, 1750–2000 euro, 2001–2500 euro, 2501–3000 euro, 3001–4000 euro, 4001 and over |
(1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10, respectively) | |||
23 | Age | Age | 18–24, 25–34, 35–44, 45–54, 55–64, 65+ years old |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
24 | Level of Education | What is the highest level of education you have completed? | Attended some primary school, Graduated from primary school, Three years of high school, Graduated from six years of high school/Lyceum, Graduated from technical school /vocational training Institute, Graduated from Technical College, Graduated from tertiary education, Completed postgraduate studies |
(1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8, respectively) |
1 | Surprises | The respondent likes surprises and always wants to be doing new things. The respondent believes that it is important to be doing many different things in their life. | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
2 | Successful | It is important for the respondent to be successful. They hope the world will acknowledge their achievements. | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
3 | Respect from others | It is important for the respondent to be respected by others and for them to comply to the respondent’s instructions. | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
4 | Pleasure in one’s actions | The respondent always seeks fun and high spirits. It is important for the respondent to do things that give them pleasure. | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) | |||
5 | Income equalization | With the help of this card, which view do you agree with most? | Scale from “Not the government’s responsibility” to “exclusively the government’s responsibility” |
(Scale from 0 to 10) | |||
6 | Incentive of Become wealthy | It is important for the respondent to be rich | Very important, Important, Somewhat important, A little important, Not important, Not at all important |
(1,2,3,4,5,6, respectively) |
PC1 | PC2 | PC3 | PC4 | PC5 | PC6 | PC7 | PC8 | PC9 | PC10 | |||
Standard deviation | 1.87 | 1.65 | 1.43 | 1.21 | 1.10 | 1.05 | 1.01 | 0.98 | 0.96 | 0.93 | ||
Proportion of Variance | 0.15 | 0.11 | 0.09 | 0.06 | 0.05 | 0.05 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.04 | ||
Cumulative Proportion | 0.15 | 0.26 | 0.35 | 0.41 | 0.46 | 0.50 | 0.55 | 0.58 | 0.62 | 0.66 | ||
PC11 | PC12 | PC13 | PC14 | PC15 | PC16 | PC17 | PC18 | PC19 | PC20 | |||
Standard deviation | 0.91 | 0.88 | 0.88 | 0.85 | 0.84 | 0.82 | 0.79 | 0.76 | 0.74 | 0.73 | ||
Proportion of Variance | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.02 | ||
Cumulative Proportion | 0.69 | 0.73 | 0.76 | 0.79 | 0.82 | 0.84 | 0.87 | 0.89 | 0.92 | 0.94 | ||
PC21 | PC22 | PC23 | PC24 | |||||||||
Standard deviation | 0.67 | 0.65 | 0.62 | 0.44 | ||||||||
Proportion of Variance | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.01 | ||||||||
Cumulative Proportion | 0.96 | 0.98 | 0.99 | 1.00 | ||||||||
PC1 | PC2 | PC3 | PC4 | PC5 | PC6 | PC7 | PC8 | PC9 | PC10 | PC11 | PC12 | |
Happiness | 0.20 | −0.20 | 0.48 | −0.22 | 0.02 | −0.03 | −0.06 | 0.06 | −0.04 | 0.25 | −0.11 | −0.13 |
Satisfaction | 0.19 | −0.18 | 0.49 | −0.23 | 0.05 | −0.04 | −0.03 | 0.05 | 0.01 | 0.27 | −0.07 | −0.14 |
State of Health | −0.32 | 0.12 | −0.16 | −0.09 | 0.11 | 0.09 | 0.19 | −0.28 | 0.10 | 0.29 | 0.09 | −0.07 |
Security | 0.09 | 0.30 | 0.10 | 0.20 | 0.15 | −0.15 | −0.01 | −0.04 | −0.35 | −0.17 | −0.19 | 0.29 |
Equitability | −0.08 | 0.27 | 0.27 | 0.23 | 0.20 | 0.11 | −0.07 | −0.01 | 0.11 | 0.02 | 0.18 | 0.01 |
Megalothymia | −0.19 | 0.25 | 0.04 | −0.26 | −0.23 | −0.09 | 0.18 | 0.23 | −0.07 | 0.00 | −0.43 | −0.05 |
Religiosity | −0.31 | −0.15 | 0.24 | −0.04 | 0.09 | −0.03 | −0.03 | 0.01 | −0.16 | −0.18 | 0.17 | 0.25 |
Tradition | 0.27 | 0.34 | 0.03 | 0.12 | −0.01 | −0.11 | 0.08 | −0.05 | −0.05 | 0.04 | −0.04 | −0.19 |
Adherence to rules | 0.23 | 0.26 | −0.10 | 0.11 | −0.21 | −0.23 | 0.16 | 0.03 | 0.09 | 0.22 | −0.05 | −0.06 |
Acknowledgment of external values | 0.18 | 0.10 | −0.01 | −0.25 | −0.46 | 0.04 | −0.02 | 0.00 | −0.20 | 0.11 | 0.43 | 0.37 |
Having a good time | −0.17 | 0.27 | 0.08 | −0.32 | 0.00 | 0.05 | 0.06 | 0.32 | 0.06 | −0.10 | −0.11 | 0.43 |
The role of migrants | −0.26 | −0.13 | 0.16 | 0.35 | −0.38 | −0.05 | 0.11 | 0.19 | −0.14 | 0.15 | −0.10 | −0.07 |
Role of the state | 0.00 | 0.06 | 0.08 | −0.22 | −0.53 | 0.14 | −0.21 | −0.37 | 0.24 | −0.40 | 0.03 | −0.22 |
Cultural change | 0.27 | 0.16 | −0.02 | −0.39 | 0.38 | 0.06 | −0.02 | −0.16 | 0.13 | −0.11 | −0.05 | 0.07 |
Altruism | 0.02 | 0.21 | 0.19 | 0.22 | −0.13 | 0.01 | −0.30 | −0.33 | 0.33 | 0.24 | −0.36 | 0.36 |
Environment | −0.02 | 0.30 | 0.29 | 0.24 | 0.04 | −0.04 | −0.05 | −0.10 | −0.09 | −0.07 | 0.32 | −0.17 |
Trust | 0.08 | −0.11 | 0.12 | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.56 | 0.45 | −0.19 | 0.16 | −0.39 | −0.02 | 0.00 |
Political self-position | −0.18 | −0.21 | 0.25 | 0.14 | −0.02 | −0.02 | 0.07 | 0.00 | 0.38 | −0.19 | −0.03 | 0.21 |
Creativity | −0.28 | 0.26 | 0.06 | −0.15 | −0.01 | 0.00 | 0.18 | 0.10 | 0.11 | 0.04 | −0.09 | −0.31 |
Freedom | −0.14 | 0.30 | 0.27 | −0.08 | 0.03 | 0.09 | −0.05 | 0.17 | −0.05 | −0.17 | 0.26 | −0.19 |
Friendship | 0.11 | −0.03 | 0.17 | 0.01 | −0.07 | 0.33 | 0.65 | −0.33 | −0.16 | 0.14 | 0.08 | 0.18 |
Income status | 0.15 | −0.02 | 0.04 | 0.17 | 0.02 | 0.65 | 0.18 | 0.11 | 0.02 | −0.26 | −0.28 | −0.09 |
Age | −0.37 | 0.04 | −0.04 | −0.14 | 0.12 | 0.01 | 0.08 | −0.21 | 0.15 | 0.27 | 0.08 | 0.01 |
Level of education | 0.23 | 0.04 | −0.04 | 0.12 | −0.01 | 0.03 | 0.18 | 0.44 | 0.57 | 0.09 | 0.29 | 0.10 |
Importance of Component: | ||||||
PC1 | PC2 | PC3 | PC4 | PC5 | PC6 | |
Standard deviation | 1.48 | 1.04 | 0.96 | 0.84 | 0.76 | 0.72 |
Proportion of Variance | 0.37 | 0.18 | 0.15 | 0.12 | 0.10 | 0.09 |
Cumulative Proportion | 0.37 | 0.55 | 0.70 | 0.82 | 0.91 | 1.00 |
PC1 | PC2 | PC3 | PC4 | PC5 | PC6 | |
Surprises | −0.46 | 0.26 | −0.41 | 0.30 | −0.37 | 0.57 |
Successful | −0.49 | −0.02 | 0.30 | 0.17 | 0.76 | 0.24 |
Respect of others | −0.35 | −0.52 | 0.46 | 0.41 | −0.44 | −0.21 |
Pleasure in ones’ activities | −0.48 | 0.19 | −0.43 | 0.02 | 0.09 | −0.73 |
Income Equalization | 0.05 | −0.78 | −0.57 | −0.02 | 0.21 | 0.13 |
Incentive of Become wealthy | −0.44 | −0.13 | 0.13 | −0.85 | −0.19 | 0.13 |
Residuals: | |||||
Min | 1Q | Median | 3Q | Max | |
−3.37 | −0.56 | −0.03 | 0.55 | 3.24 | |
Coefficients: | |||||
Estimate | Std. Error | t value | Pr(>|t|) | ||
(Intercept) | −1.34 | 0.09 | −14.39 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Magnanimity | 0.33 | 0.02 | 18.55 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Tradition | 0.04 | 0.02 | 1.99 | 0.05 | * |
Acknowledgment of external values | −0.06 | 0.02 | −3.31 | 0.00 | *** |
Having a good time | 0.21 | 0.02 | 10.86 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Signif. codes: 0—“***”; 0.001—“**”; 0.01—“*”; 0.05—“.”; 0.1—“ ”. 1. Residual standard error: 0.8078 on 1278 degrees of freedom. Multiple R-squared: 0.3495; adjusted R-squared: 0.3474. F-statistic: 171.6 on 4 and 1278 DF; p-value: <2.2 × 10−16.
Residuals: | |||||
Min | 1Q | Median | 3Q | Max | |
−2.99 | −0.50 | −0.10 | 0.51 | 3.31 | |
Coefficients: | |||||
Estimate | Std. Error | t value | Pr(>|t|) | ||
(Intercept) | −1.39 | 0.12 | −11.27 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Happiness | −0.06 | 0.01 | −4.85 | 0.00 | *** |
Magnanimity | 0.09 | 0.02 | 5.10 | 0.00 | *** |
Having a good time | 0.37 | 0.02 | 18.67 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Cultural change | 0.02 | 0.01 | 2.12 | 0.03 | * |
Environment | 0.19 | 0.02 | 7.76 | 0.00 | *** |
Political self-definition | 0.03 | 0.01 | 2.78 | 0.01 | ** |
Signif. codes: 0—“***”; 0.001—“**”; 0.01—“*”; 0.05—“.”; 0.1—“ ”. 1. Residual standard error: 0.8112 on 1276 degrees of freedom. Multiple R-squared: 0.345; Adjusted R-squared: 0.3419. F-statistic: 112 on 6 and 1276 DF; p-value: <2.2 × 10−16.
Residuals: | |||||
Min | 1Q | Median | 3Q | Max | |
−2.74 | −0.58 | 0.02 | 0.57 | 2.27 | |
Coefficients: | |||||
Estimate | Std. Error | t value | Pr(>|t|) | ||
(Intercept) | −1.20 | 0.12 | −10.11 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Magnanimity | 0.22 | 0.02 | 11.45 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Tradition | −0.08 | 0.02 | −3.90 | 0.00 | *** |
Acknowledgment of external values | 0.05 | 0.02 | 2.72 | 0.01 | ** |
Having a good time | 0.27 | 0.02 | 13.16 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Cultural change | 0.03 | 0.01 | 3.21 | 0.00 | ** |
Trust | −0.03 | 0.01 | −2.37 | 0.02 | * |
Friendship | −0.05 | 0.02 | −2.45 | 0.01 | * |
Signif. codes: 0—“***”; 0.001—“**”; 0.01—“*”; 0.05—“.”; 0.1—“ ”. 1. Residual standard error: 0.8435 on 1275 degrees of freedom. Multiple R-squared: 0.2925; Adjusted R-squared: 0.2886. F-statistic: 75.29 on 7 and 1275 DF; p-value: <2.2 × 10−16.
Residuals: | |||||
Min | 1Q | Median | 3Q | Max | |
−2.43 | −0.61 | −0.09 | 0.51 | 3.24 | |
Coefficients: | |||||
Estimate | Std. Error | t value | Pr(>|t|) | ||
(Intercept) | −0.82 | 0.12 | −6.93 | 0.00 | *** |
Happiness | −0.06 | 0.01 | −5.15 | 0.00 | *** |
Magnanimity | 0.19 | 0.02 | 9.41 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Having a good time | 0.19 | 0.02 | 8.93 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Environment | 0.12 | 0.03 | 4.56 | 0.00 | *** |
Signif. codes: 0—“***”; 0.001—“**”; 0.01—“*”; 0.05—“.”; 0.1—“ ”. 1. Residual standard error: 0.8875 on 1278 degrees of freedom. Multiple R-squared: 0.2148; Adjusted R-squared: 0.2124. F-statistic: 87.42 on 4 and 1278 DF; p-value: <2.2 × 10−16.
Residuals: | |||||
Min | 1Q | Median | 3Q | Max | |
−2.39 | −0.62 | −0.22 | 0.53 | 3.79 | |
Coefficients: | |||||
Estimate | Std. Error | t value | Pr(>|t|) | ||
(Intercept) | −1.31 | 0.08 | −16.04 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Magnanimity | 0.17 | 0.02 | 8.30 | 0.00 | *** |
Tradition | 0.18 | 0.02 | 8.25 | 0.00 | *** |
Having a good time | 0.18 | 0.02 | 8.47 | <2 × 10−16 | *** |
Signif. codes: 0—“***”; 0.001—“**”; 0.01—“*”; 0.05—“.”; 0.1—“ ”. 1. Residual standard error: 0.9047 on 1279 degrees of freedom. Multiple R-squared: 0.1834; Adjusted R-squared: 0.1815. F-statistic: 95.78 on 3 and 1279 DF; p-value: <2.2 × 10−16.
Variables of Incentives | R2 | |
---|---|---|
1 | Successful | 0.35 |
2 | Pleasure in Activities | 0.34 |
3 | Become wealthy | 0.29 |
4 | Surprises | 0.21 |
5 | Respect from others | 0.18 |
Successful | Pleasure in What One Does | Become Wealthy | Surprises | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Basic Conditions | (-) Happiness | (-) Happiness | ||
Political Self-position | ||||
Basic Values | Tradition | (-)Tradition | ||
Environment | Environment | |||
(-) Acknowledgement of External Values | Acknowledgement of External Values | |||
(-) Τrust | ||||
(-) Friendship | ||||
Dynamism | Megalothymia | Megalothymia | Megalothymia | Megalothymia |
Having a good time | Having a good time | Having a good time | Having a good time | |
Cultural Change |
© 2021 Copyright by the authors. Licensed as an open access article using a CC BY 4.0 license.